Color Vision: Perception in the Retina, Part 1

How do we really see colors?

           How do we perceive colors?  Ever questioned how so many people can be colorblind, yet still see perfectly?  Recently, in the article Time to Get Real: The Riddle of Perception, by The Huffington Post, our unique ability to perceive color is touched upon. After reading it, I felt it necessary to spend the time to explain to you just how interesting the phenomenon of color perception actually is, and how “color” perception actually comes second to shapes.

Our ability to see a range of colors (although quite small) is the result of a series of stages of processing, involving both feedback and feedforward mechanisms. Light (an electric signal) from the external world gets converted into a chemical signal by neurons in our Retinas, and are processed through pathways in the brain. These subsequent processing stages, where our neurons “detect” edges and borders by comparing contrast, eventually result in the perception of both objects and colors.

Primitively, discriminating between different objects of varying size, shape, and color was necessary for survival, especially in determining ripeness of food. Scientists have believe there to be an ancient universal color system among Vertebrates, in which all had 1 pigment gene with a absorption <500nm, one with an absorption >500nm, and Rhodopsin (Nathans, 1999).This primitive system has evolved through either gene duplication or a polymorphism, and resulted in the different species with different numbers of pigment genes, leading to the detection of a spectrum of colors. Therefore color vision, while originally meant for determining basic contrast differences for survival, has evolved to enhance our perception of objects in world. The ability of Humans to see visible light completely depends on only four different types of pigment genes that respond best to four wavelengths of light: Red (559nm), Green(531nm), and Blue(419nm), and Rhodopsin (496nm) (Nathans, 1999).

I have decided to explain this phenomenon of shape and color perception in two parts. In the first part, I will explain how neurons in the Retina perceive different hues, and how the activation of only a few types of cells results in our perception of a spectrum of colors.  In the second part, I will explain how physics actually comes into play, and directly results in a sad, but necessary trade-off between excellent perception of shapes, and lack of perception of a larger spectrum of colors.

Neurons in the Retina Compare Signals 

The retina consists of four different types of wavelength-sensitive sensory receptors. These sensory receptors are called Photoreceptors. Photoreceptors activate (or hyperpolarize, to be exact) when a photon of it’s preferred wavelength contacts it. This results in a cascade of events, that eventually, upon reaching higher processing levels in the cortex, results in our perception of objects, colors, and space.

The four types of photoreceptors can be split into two groups: Rods and Cones. Neurons called Bipolar Cells compare the inputs from different Rods and Cones in order to distinguish different hues. After that, neurons called Ganglion Cells further compare the bipolar cell inputs, and send information to the brain, as the 1st of many steps towards perceiving the visual world around us.

From Light to Neural Info

Rods are larger and more numerous than cones. Rods use the ancient pigment gene, Rhodopsin, and are not very sensitive to light (it take 10 photons for a response), but once activated, they can take up to an hour to return to normal levels in order to capture more photons. Therefore, having 120 million of them-compared to 3 million Cones-is quite useful.  Rods saturate (reach maximal sensitivity) around 496nm, making them good for detecting objects in dim light levels and darkness (Naarendorp, 2010). Their low saturation level and their abundance in the outer retina makes them more useful for luminance instead of shape perception (spatial acuity)…I will explain more about this in Part 2.

On the other hand, Human Cones respond to longer wavelengths, and never saturate, making them useful to perceive colors, objects, and the visual world. There are three types of Cone pigment genes, or Cones: L Cones (Long wavelength or “red” sensitive), M Cones (Medium wavelength or “green” sensitive), and S Cones (Short wavelength or “blue” sensitive). Cones are abundant in center of the retina (the fovea) to about 10 degrees out from the center of vision. Cones are therefore what we use in normal and bright light conditions and for visual acuity (seeing things clearly) (Li, 2010).

Comparing Different Signals Through Processing Levels

This interesting task of comparing wavelengths in order to perceive the world is possible because of Receptive Fields. The idea of receptive fields, came about in the late 1800’s when scientists realized our retinas couldn’t have separate pigment genes for different colors that compared the colors in later stages of processing. This became clear when subjects were presented with a white screen after focusing on colored circles and the afterimage (or complementary color) appeared. If separate pathways were used for separate colors, the same color should be perceived on a white screen, not the inverse color.

If you were to make a cylinder by curling your fingers to touch your thumb and look through it, closing one eye, this is analogous to a single cells receptive field. Cones and Rods respond this way. When the correct stimulus enters that circle, the Rod or Cone will activate (for our purposes activation=1). If an incorrect stimulus (not the optimal wavelength) hits, the Rod or Cone will not activate (for our purposes, not activating=0).

Screen Shot 2013-03-04 at 12.52.01 PM

Now, imagine a smaller circle in the center of that big circle. This is analogous to the receptive field of a Bipolar Cell. A Bipolar cell gets inputs to the center of that circle from a single type of Rod or Cone, and gets input to the surround (the large encompassing circle minus the center circle) from a mix of differing types of Rods and/or Cones. Also, that center circle has a higher gain than the surround, meaning that instead of 1’s and 0’s, lets say 2’s activate the center, whereas the surround continues to activate to 1’s or 0’s.

The Red-Green Pathway

For example, if a  L ON/ M OFF Bipolar Cell is presented with circle of red light, it would activate maximally, but as green light starts to surround that red circle, the Bipolar Cell would begin to activate less and less, summing the center + inputs from the red and the – surround inputs from the green and firing respectively. At the same time, a neighboring M ON/ L OFF Bipolar Cell, would not fire at all to the initial red circle of light, for its receptive field would only be receiving – L (Red) inputs, but as the green light builds up in the  surround receptive field of the other Bipolar Cell, it encroaches on the center of the neighbor Bipolar Cells receptive field, subsequently adding +M, and initiating greater and greater firing.  This pathway is defined as the Red-Green Axis in Color-Opponency Theory. 

What about Blue?

Information from blue cones travels in a separate path to the 3rd stage of retinal processing (Ganglion Cells), but also joins with the information from Red and Green to create the Blue-Yellow Axis. Certain Bipolar Cells take information from a mix of Red and Green in the surround and from Blue in the center. The mix of Red + Green is defined as Yellow,and is subtracted by the center Blue (+2) responses.

Bringing it All Together

     These Different Bipolar Cells compare different inputs from different numbers and types of photoreceptors. Some Bipolar Cells (those in the fovea) get input from a single cone to it’s center, and from 10 or so cones to it’s surround. Others, in the periphery, get input from up to 5 cones in their center, and many more in their surround. Other helper cells (Horizontal and Amacrine Cells) help inhibit certain signals and certain times from certain photoreceptors to help enhance the reliability of the message passed along to Ganglion Cells.

      Where Bipolar’s mix and match these three optimal wavelengths (Red, Green, and Blue) at a local level (single Bipolar Cells) the differences in firing of each of the Bipolar Cells on a global level (the entire retina) is compared by Ganglion Cells. Ganglion’s then transmit this information to the brain for reconstruction of the visual world (Erikoz, 2010). From here, perception of shapes and hues in our visual scene finally begins. 

Advertisements